Edición Marzo 2020
La investigación cualitativa y sus métodos a través de las citas literales de los mejores autores
Las fuentes de procedencia de las citas literales, como (p.e.) en Denzin y Lincoln 2011, o en Sarah J. Tracy 2016 hacen referencia a la Bibliografía disponible en la página sobre Investigación Cualitativa.
Fuente: Denzin y Lincoln 2011
Qualitative research is a situated activity that locates the observer in the world.
(…)
Qualitative research consists of a set of interpretive, material practices that make the world visible. These practices transform the world. They turn the world into a series of representations, including fieldnotes, interviews, conversations, photographs, recordings, and memos to the self. At this level, qualitative research involves an interpretive, naturalistic approach to the world. This means that qualitative researchers study things in their natural settings, attempting to make sense of or interpret phenomena in terms of the meanings people bring to them.
Qualitative research involves the studied use and collection of a variety of empirical materials— case study, personal experience, introspection, life story, interview, artifacts, and cultural texts and productions, along with observational, historical, interactional, and visual texts— that describe routine and problematic moments and meanings in individuals’ lives.
(…)
The combination of multiple methodological practices, empirical materials, perspectives, and observers in a single study is best understood, then, as a strategy that adds rigor, breadth complexity, richness, and depth to any inquiry
(…)
The word qualitative implies an emphasis on the qualities of entities and on processes and meanings that are not experimentally examined or measured (if measured at all) in terms of quantity, amount, intensity, or frequency.
(…)
Proponents of the positivist version contend that there is a reality out there to be studied, captured, and understood, whereas the postpositivists argue that reality can never be fully apprehended, only approximated (Guba, 1990a, p. 22). Postpositivism relies on multiple methods as a way of capturing as much of reality as possible.
(…)
Qualitative researchers believe that rich descriptions of the social world are valuable, whereas quantitative researchers, with their etic, nomothetic commitments, are less concerned with such detail. They are deliberately unconcerned with such descriptions because such detail interrupts the process of developing generalizations.
(…)
Qualitative researchers use ethnographic prose, historical narratives, first-person accounts, still photographs, life history, fictionalized “facts,” and biographical and autobiographical materials, among others. Quantitative researchers use mathematical models, statistical tables, and graphs and usually write in an impersonal, third-person prose.
(…)
We want a social science committed up front to issues of social justice, equity, nonviolence, peace, and universal human rights. We do not want a social science that says it can address these issues if it wants to do so. For us, this is no longer an option.

Fuente: Miles, Huberman y Saldaña 2014
We label ourselves pragmatic realists. We believe that social phenomena exist not only in the mind but also in the world— and that some reasonably stable relationships can be found among the idiosyncratic messiness of life.
(…)
Saldaña (2011b) describes more than 20 different qualitative research genres out of many more available to investigators, ranging from well-established traditions such as ethnography, grounded theory, phenomenology, case study, and content analysis to more progressive genres of qualitative research, such as poetic inquiry, narrative inquiry, ethnodrama, autoethnography, and duoethnography.
(…)
- Qualitative research is conducted through intense and/ or prolonged contact with participants in a naturalistic setting to investigate the everyday and/ or exceptional lives of individuals, groups, societies, and organizations.
- The researcher’s role is to gain a holistic (systemic, encompassing, and integrated) overview of the context under study: its social arrangement, its ways of working, and its explicit and implicit rules.
- Relatively little standardized instrumentation is used. The researcher himself or herself is essentially the main instrument in the study.
- The researcher attempts to capture data on the perceptions of local participants from the inside through a process of deep attentiveness, of empathetic understanding, and of suspending or bracketing preconceptions about the topics under discussion.
- Most of the analysis is done with words. The words can be assembled, subclustered, or broken into segments. They can be reorganized to permit the researcher to compare, contrast, analyze, and construct patterns out of them.
- Reading through these empirical materials (i.e., data), the researcher may construct certain themes and patterns that can be reviewed with participants.
- The main task is to describe the ways people in particular settings come to understand, account for, take action, and otherwise manage their day-to-day situations.
- Many interpretations of this material are possible, but some are more compelling for theoretical reasons or on grounds of credibility and trustworthiness.
(…)
Research questions represent the facets of inquiry that the researcher most wants to explore. Research questions may be general or particular, descriptive or explanatory. The formulation of research questions may precede, follow, or happen concurrently with the development of a conceptual framework. They also may be formulated at the outset or later on and may be refined or reformulated during the course of fieldwork.
(…)
Formulating more than a dozen or so general research questions is looking for trouble. You can easily lose the forest for the trees and fragment the collection of data. Having a large number of questions makes it harder to see emergent links across different parts of the database and to integrate findings. As we saw in Display 2.4, a solution to research question proliferation is the use of major questions, each with subquestions, for clarity and specificity. It also helps to consider whether there is a key question, the “thing you really want to know.”
(…)
Qualitative samples tend to be purposive rather than random. Samples in qualitative studies are usually not wholly prespecified but can evolve once fieldwork begins. The initial choices of participants lead you to similar and different ones; observing one class of events invites comparison with another; and understanding one key relationship in the setting reveals facets to be studied in others. This is conceptually driven sequential sampling.
(…)
Random sampling is a gold standard of quantitative research but is used quite minimally in qualitative research because random sampling can sometimes deal you a biased hand. Our sampling tends to be more strategic and purposive because we are focusing on a case’s unique contexts. Admittedly, there are times when we select a case to study because it is accessible to us geographically and immediately— a form of convenience sampling.
How do sampling strategies affect analysis? Maximum variation sampling, for example, involves looking for outlier cases to see whether the main patterns still hold, while homogeneous sampling focuses on people with similar demographic or social characteristics. The critical case is the instance that “proves” or exemplifies the main findings. Searching deliberately for confirming and disconfirming cases, extreme or deviant cases, and typical cases serves to increase confidence in conclusions.
Some strategies benefit inductive, theory-building analysis (e.g., opportunistic or snowball sampling). Politically important cases are “salient” participants who may need to be included (or excluded) because they connect with politically sensitive issues anticipated in the analysis.
(…)
The conventional view is that qualitative studies are only good for exploratory or descriptive forays and that strong explanations, including causal attributions, can be derived only through quantitative studies, particularly the classical experimental– control design. We consider this view mistaken. Even the most elegant quantitative procedures deal primarily with associations, not really causes. They can only develop plausible possibilities smoothed across many persons and situations. We consider qualitative analysis to be a very powerful method for assessing causation. But we have to go far beyond assertion, showing an empirical basis for the claim that Y is explained or caused/ influenced by X.

Fuente: Shkedi 2014
Literature in the field of qualitative research that seeks to define this realm presents a list of dozens of significant terms, some of which are parallel, if not identical. Among the terms we can randomly find are “case study,” “action research,” clinical research,” “ethnography,” “content analysis,” “discourse analysis,” “descriptive study,” “discussion analysis,” “documental analysis,” “ethnography,” “field research,” “focus group,” “grounded theory,” “hermeneutics,” “interpretive research,” “narrative research,” “life history,” “life stories,” “naturalistic research,” “participant observation,” “qualitative assessment,” “phenomenology,” “constructivist research,” “post-modern research,” “biography,” and “autobiography,” to name just a few. Tesch (1990) presented perhaps the most comprehensive list, numbering over 40 different types of qualitative research. Each of the terms reflects different emphases, traditions, attitudes and preferences. Some of the terms actually parallel one another; others present different variants of the same or close types of research, while some probably reflect disagreement or controversies between different researchers.
(…)
Guba and Lincoln (1998) suggest a distinction between four qualitative research approaches: positivist, post-positivist, critical and constructive. Although we can certainly propose additional approaches, it appears that these four approaches reflect the range of qualitative research paradigm references. Frequently accepted is the dichotomous distinction between positivist research and constructivist research.
(…)
Many qualitative researchers conduct their research under a non-constructivist approach, while many others are very loyal to the constructive approach. The distinction between the different research approaches, in all its importance, cannot provide us the key for a clear distinction between the different practices of qualitative research. Researchers who adopt the constructivist approach may conduct research via life stories or ethnography, action research or case studies, a grounded theory or discourse analysis, to mention only a very partial list. But researchers who adopt the post-positivist approach, the critical approach or even the positivist approach may also conduct their research within the research genres mentioned above.
(…)
We can sort the terms that identify qualitative research into four “families,” each of which reflects a different aspect of qualitative research: the research approach, genre, method and methodology. These represent four different ways of looking at four different faces of qualitative research.
(…)
Although in most cases, researchers tend to identify their research with one of the accepted genres, this identification does not make it easier to distinguish between the different types of studies. While some researchers would categorize their research type as ethnography, it could be referred to by others as case study, while other scholars would call it narrative research, still others would prefer to call it life story, and so on. Identification with a particular research genre is largely a reflection of the research tradition in which the researcher has grown or developed. Moreover, even if some researchers use the same genre name, each may conduct a different study. Some researchers may choose to pursue ethnography which focuses on a post-positivistic approach, and others base their ethnography on constructivist or critical approaches.
(…)
Denzin and Lincoln (2000) proposed a comprehensive definition of qualitative research: «Qualitative research is a situated activity that locates the observer in the world. […] This means that qualitative researchers study things in their natural settings, attempting to make sense of, or to interpret, phenomena in terms of the meanings people bring to them.» (p. 3) Another definition, which partially overlaps the definition above, was offered by Maykut and Morehouse (1994): «Qualitative research […] generally examines people’s words and actions in narrative or descriptive ways more closely representing the situation as experienced by the participants.» (p. 2) Strauss and Corbin (1990) suggested a broader definition: «By the term qualitative research we mean any kind of research that produces findings not arrived at by means of statistical procedures or other means of quantification.”
These three definitions, and certainly many others, identify qualitative research, first and foremost, via terminological descriptions, with closeness to the phenomena under study in its natural environment, and by avoiding the use of sophisticated statistical-quantitative tools. It seems that these definitions emphasize the methodological aspects, which are indeed the main characteristics of qualitative research.
Based upon these and other definitions of qualitative research, this book offers a definition based on the following three components:
- Research based on intuitive human research skills, focused on closeness, participation, and empathy with the phenomena investigated.
- The use of analytic human research skills, focused on distancing, reflection and control of the process.
- Research utilizing the language of words, a natural human language in the context of the natural human life.
Fuente: Trochim, 2006 [accesible online: bit.ly/2bJ78uJ]
Depending on their philosophical perspectives, some qualitative researchers reject the framework of validity that is commonly accepted in more quantitative research in the social sciences. They reject the basic realist assumption that their is a reality external to our perception of it. Consequently, it doesn’t make sense to be concerned with the «truth» or «falsity» of an observation with respect to an external reality (which is a primary concern of validity). These qualitative researchers argue for different standards for judging the quality of research.
For instance, Guba and Lincoln proposed four criteria for judging the soundness of qualitative research and explicitly offered these as an alternative to more traditional quantitatively-oriented criteria. They felt that their four criteria better reflected the underlying assumptions involved in much qualitative research. Their proposed criteria and the «analogous» quantitative criteria are listed in the table.
Traditional Criteria for Judging Quantitative Research | Alternative Criteria for Judging Qualitative Research |
internal validity | credibility |
external validity | transferability |
reliability | dependability |
objectivity | confirmability |

Fuente: David Williams, 2016
Credibility
The credibility standard requires a qualitative study to be believable to critical readers and to be approved by the persons who provided the information gathered during the study. Lincoln and Guba recommend several techniques inquirers may use to enhance the credibility of their research: prolonged engagement, persistent observation, triangulation, peer debriefing, negative case analysis, progressive subjectivity checks, and member checking.
(…)
Transferability
This criterion refers to the applicability of findings in one context (where the research is done) to other contexts or settings (where the interpretations might be transferred). Whether findings can be transferred or not is an empirical question, which cannot be answered by the inquirer alone. The target context must be compared to the research context to identify similarities. The more similar, the more likely it is that the findings will be transferable. Persons reading the qualitative inquiry reports have to make this decision.
(…)
Confirmability
A fourth standard is confirmability, which refers to the quality of the results produced by an inquiry in terms of how well they are supported by informants (members) who are involved in the study and by events that are independent of the inquirer. Reference to literature and findings by other authors that confirm the inquirer’s interpretations can strengthen confirmability of the study in addition to information and interpretations by people other than the inquirer from within the inquiry site itself.
(…)
Dependability
This is the third standard for judging qualitative studies and refers to the stability or consistency of the inquiry processes used over time. To check the dependability of a qualitative study, one looks to see if the researcher has been careless or made mistakes in conceptualizing the study, collecting the data, interpreting the findings and reporting results. The logic used for selecting people and events to observe, interview, and include in the study should be clearly presented. The more consistent the researcher has been in this research process, the more dependable are the results.
Fuente: C. H. Major; M. Savin-Baden, 2010
(Del apartado Qualitative research synthesis. Top ten arguments for using the approach (pp. 10-16))
Qualitative research synthesis:
- Can help contain the information explosion
- Helps manage the information explosion
- Helps address the problem of knowledge fragmentation
- Helps to identify gaps and omissions in a given body of research or within a single article
- Provides a different perspective on questions addressed through quantitative approaches
- Provides ways to advance theory
- Can spark dialogue and debate
- Can add a depth dimension to qualitative studies
- Can allow for development of evidence-based practice and policy
- Is a cost-efficient approach
Fuente: Sarah J. Tracy, 2016
Qualitative quality: creating a credible, ethical, significant study (Cap. 11)
(…)
Hence I developed an expansive «big-tent» approach to criteria for qualitative quality, differentiating the end goals of good research from the mean practices that researchers take to get there. This eight-point conceptualization for obtaining quality in qualitative research serves as a pedagogical tool, promotes dialogue among researchers from various paradigms, and encourages the viability and credibility of qualitative research with a variety of audiences.
(…)
Eight «big-tent» criteria for excellent qualitative research (Sarah J. Tracy, 2016)
Criteria for Quality (end goal) | Various means, practices and methods through which to achieve |
Worthy topic | The topic if the research is:
|
Rich rigor | The study uses sufficient, abundant, appropriate and complex
|
Sincerity | The study is characterized by
|
Credibility | The research is marked by
|
Resonance | The research influences, affects, or moves particulars readers or a variety of audiences through
|
Significant contribution | The research provides a significant contribution
|
Ethical | The research considers
|
Meaningful coherence | The study
|
Fuente: Ben Flyvbjerg, 2013
(…)
Case studies have been around as long as recorded history and today they account for a large proportion of books and articles in psychology, anthropology, sociology, history, political science, education, economics, management, biology, and medical science. For instance, in recent years roughly half of all articles in the top political science journals have used case studies, according to Alexandr George and Andrew Bennet (2005, pp. 4-5). Much of what we know about the empirical world has been produced by case study research, and many of the most treasured classics in each discipline are case studies.
(…)
One can often generalize on the basis of a single case, and the case study may be central to scientific development via generalization as supplement or alternative to other methods. But formal generalization is overvalued as a source of scientific development, whereas «the force of example» and transferability are underestimated.
(…)
The case study is useful for both generating and testing of hypotheses but is not limited to these research activities alone.
(…)
Testing of hypotheses relates directly to the question of «generalizability», and this in turn relates to the question of case selection. Here, generalizability of case studies can be increased by the strategic selection of cases.
Type of Selection | Purpose |
Information oriented selection | To maximize the utility of information from small samples and single cases. Cases are selected on the basis of expectations about their information content. [Subtipos indicados a continuación] |
Extreme/deviant case | To obtain information on unusual cases, which can be especially problematic or especially good in a more closely defined sense. To understand the limits of existing theories and to develop new concepts, variables, and theories that are able to account for deviant cases. |
Maximun variation cases | To obtain information about the significance of various circumstances for case process and outcome; e.g., three to four cases that are very different on one dimension: size, form of organization, location, budget, et c. |
Critical cases | To achieve information that permits logical deductions of the type, «If this is (not) valid for this case, then it applies to all (no) cases». |
Paradigmatic cases | To develop a metaphor or stablish a school for the domain that the case concerns. |
Fuente: Uwe Flick, 2017
Sometimes it is doubted whether sampling is the right term if you do qualitative research (e.g. Maxwell, 2013, p. 96). But in qualitative research, we of course face the problem addressed with this term as well: we have to select the ‘right’ cases, groups, and materials in a way that is somehow defined -so that we can do our study with limited resources, from a more or less infinite horizon of possible selections. And with what we select, we want to make statements that we can generalize in one way or the other- in most cases at least beyond the research situations and beyond the four or 40 people we interviewed for example.
Fuente: Michael Quinn Patton, 2002
Sampling Strategies – Purposeful sampling (p. 243)
Type | Purpose |
Extreme or deviant case | Learning from unusual manifestation of the phenomenon of interest, for example, outstanding successes/notable failures; top of the class/dropouts; exotic events, crises. |
Intensity sampling | Information-rich cases that manifest the phenomenon intensely, but not extremely, for example, good students/poor students; above average/below average. |
Maximum variation sampling -purposefully picking a wide range of cases to get variation on dimensions of interest | Document unique or diverse variations that have emerged in adapting to different conditions. Identify important common patterns that cut across variations (cut through the noise of variation). |
Homogeneous sampling | Focus; reduce variation; simplify analysis; facilitate group interviewing. |
Typical case sampling | Illustrate or highlight what is typical, normal, average. |
Critical case sampling | Permits logical generalization and maximum application of information to other cases because if it’s true of this one case, it’s likely to other cases. |
Snowball or chain sampling | Identify cases of interest from sampling people who know people who what cases are information rich, that is, good example for study, good interview participants. |
Criterion sampling | Picking all cases that meet some criterion, for example, all children abused in a treatment facility. Quality assurance. |
Theory-based sampling, operational construct sampling, or theoretical sampling | Finding manifestations of a theoretical construct of interest so as to elaborate and examine the construct and its variations. |
Confirming and disconfirming cases | Elaborating and deepening initial analyses; seeking exceptions; testing variations. |
Stratified purposeful sampling | Illustrate characteristics of particular subgroups of interest; facilitate comparisons. |
Opportunistic or emergent sampling | Following new leads during fieldwork; taking advantage of the unexpected; flexibility. |
Purposeful random sampling (still small sample size) | Add credibility when potential purposeful sample is larger than one can handle. Reduces bias within a purposeful category (Not for genralizations or representativeness.) |
Sampling politically important cases | Attract attention to the study (or avoid attracting undesired attention by purposefully eliminating from the sample politically sensitive cases). |
Convenience sampling | Do what’s easy to save time, money, and effort. Poorest rationale; lowest credibility. Yields information-poor cases. |
Combination or mixed purposeful sampling | Triangulation; flexibility; meet multiple interests and needs. |
Source: Patton, M.Q., 2002
Fuente: Bernard, Wutich, Ryan, 2016
Sample Size in Nonprobability Sampling
The problem of sample size is not quite as well understood when it comes to ethnography, grounded theory, schema analysis, narrative analysis, and the like, but a lot of progress is being made and new research is coming out all the time (Guest 2015). Our advice—based on this emerging evidence—is that 20–60 knowledgeable people are enough to uncover and understand core themes.
To explain our recommendation, we’ll walk you through a few different areas of research. The first has to do with how many interviews it takes to detect the simple existence of a theme in research on a fairly focused phenomenon, with a reasonably homogenous sample. Studies of this kind focus on well-defined cultural domains (like the list of things you can put in a salad or the list of ways to treat a cold) or on lived experience (like coping with a particular illness or surviving combat or being out of work). (See Chapter 18 for more on collecting and analyzing data about cultural domains. See Chapters 10 and 13 on studying lived experience.)
Morgan et al. (2002:76) did in-depth interviews with four different samples of people about various risks in the environment. The researchers did the usual coding for concepts, but they also plotted the cumulative number of new concepts identified after each interview across the four different studies. Their results are in Figure 3.1. In every case, the shape of the line is the same: The first few interviews produce a lot of new data, but by 20 interviews, the curves flatten out and hardly any new information is retrieved.
Tipología de artículos científicos según las Normas APA (APA Style)
Types of Articles and Papers
- Quantitative Articles
- Qualitative Articles
- Mixed Methods Articles
- Replication Articles
- Quantitative and Qualitative Meta-Analyses
- Literature Review Articles
- Theoretical Articles
- Methodological Articles
- Other Types of Articles
- Student Papers, Dissertations, and Theses
Fuente: https://apastyle.apa.org/products/publication-manual-7th-edition/index?tab=2
Fuente: Major, C.H; Savin-Baden, M. 2010
APA Literature Review Articles. APA Style 7h ed. 2020
Literature Review Articles (or narrative literature review articles) provide narrative summaries and evaluations of the findings or theories within a literature base. The literature base may include qualitative, quantitative, and/or mixed methods research. Literature review capture trends in the literature; they not engage in a systematic quantitative o qualitative meta-analysis of the findings from the initial studies.
In literature review articles, authors should
- define and clarify the problem;
- summarize previous investigations to inform readers of the state of the research;
- identify relations, contradictions, gaps, and inconsistencies in the literature and
- suggest next steps in solving the problem.
The components of the literature review articles can be arranged in various ways -for example, by grouping research on the basis of similarity in the concepts or theories of the interest, methodological similarities among the studies reviewed, or the historical development of the field.
Source: APA (2020). Publication Manual of the American Psychological Association. Washington, APA, pag. 8.
Ir a la página principal sobre Investigación cualitativa